Search

From lobsters to honey bees, social distancing is common in the animal kingdom - Science Magazine

sanirbanir.blogspot.com

Caribbean spiny lobsters use chemical cues in urine to identify sick lobsters.

Stephen Frink Collection/Alamy Stock Photo

“Social distancing” has become one of the buzz phrases of the year. But it turns out humans aren’t the only animals that put some space between themselves and others to reduce the transmission of disease. Wildlife—from finches to mandrills—use similar tactics, according to a paper published this week in the Proceedings of the Royal Society B.

Science chatted with two of the study’s authors—Andrea Townsend, a behavioral ecologist at Hamilton College, and Dana Hawley, a biologist at the Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University—about how self-isolating works throughout the animal kingdom.

This interview has been edited for clarity and length.

Q: How do animals know when they need to socially distance?

Andrea Townsend: COVID-19 has so many symptoms that it’s hard to know when someone is sick. But some animals like house finches use very general behavioral cues, such as lethargy, to assess potential infections and avoid certain individuals.

Dana Hawley: In other cases, animals have evolved fairly complex cues to induce social distancing. The Caribbean spiny lobster [a social lobster that normally lives in groups] has evolved to detect a chemical cue in the urine of sick lobsters and avoid areas that these sick lobsters occupy.

Another example is in mandrills. Researchers took the feces of animals that did or did not have parasites and basically put a tiny amount on the side of a tree. They found that the primates were much more strongly drawn to the feces of unparasitized animals than those that were parasitized.

Andrea Townsend's current research focuses on how land use changes affect wild birds.

Nancy L. Ford/Hamilton College

Q: What does it look like when animals social distance?

D.H.: For the spiny lobsters, their social behavior is being packed together in dens, which provide them with a lot of protection. For them, social distancing means abandoning that den, which is quite a dangerous prospect for them. In other cases, it’s a lot more subtle, like just interacting less with a particular individual in a group.

Q: There also seem to be some examples of animals self-quarantining.

D.H.: The best examples of what we might call “quarantining” come from social insects like ants and bees. In some cases, infected insects will purposefully leave the colony to go off and die.

A.T.: In these types of insect colonies, individuals are highly related to one another—more so than in human family groups. So, it can be to the benefit of an individual to sacrifice itself to protect its larger family.

Q: How do we know they’re social distancing or quarantining because of disease, and not for some other reason?

Dana Hawley’s work explores the relationship between non-human animal social behavior and disease. 

Virginia Tech

D.H.: There are several ways of getting at the cause of these behaviors. One neat way that can be done in wild animals is to medicate individuals to remove the parasites and then see whether the avoidance behavior changes. Researchers did that in the mandrills. Even though parasitized individuals were avoided previously, they found that once they were treated for the infection, they began being groomed again.

Researchers have also done great studies in ants where they introduced a fungal pathogen into a colony, and then compared the behavioral response to colonies where a water solution was introduced as a control. If you see changes in behavior [limiting movement within the nest or spending more time outside that nest] that are consistent with avoidance of the pathogen, that’s pretty strong evidence that those animals are responding to the fungus itself.

Q: But animals can’t always social distance, right?

A.T.: One of my favorite examples is the Tasmanian devil. It has this really nasty transmissible cancer that is spread through biting. You would think that they should just stop biting one another, but biting is really important for their mating: They bite rivals over mates, and then they even bite while they’re mating.

Q: Any lessons for us?

D.H.: For me, a big takeaway is that social distancing works. Anytime we see a behavior that has evolved again and again in unrelated types of animals, that’s a signal that even though social distancing is a very costly behavior, the benefits clearly outweigh the costs.

Let's block ads! (Why?)



"social" - Google News
August 14, 2020 at 07:00PM
https://ift.tt/2PQtCQC

From lobsters to honey bees, social distancing is common in the animal kingdom - Science Magazine
"social" - Google News
https://ift.tt/38fmaXp
https://ift.tt/2WhuDnP

Bagikan Berita Ini

0 Response to "From lobsters to honey bees, social distancing is common in the animal kingdom - Science Magazine"

Post a Comment

Powered by Blogger.